Happiness and sadness are emotions, which are states. Although there may
be structural differences in a person who is depressed (more prone to be in a
“sad state”) versus a brain who has a more balanced tendency to exist in a state
(a euthymic person), only functional neuroimaging can be used to study
differences in brains between a “happy” and a “sad state. This is challenging,
as happiness and sadness are emotions that are subjective, and inducing
emotions in the setting of an MRI or PET is also difficult. Furthermore fMRI
and PET scans measure metabolic changes in brain regions, meaning that they
only demonstrate changes in the time frame of seconds to minutes after a state
is induced. Now to the neuroscience of emotion.
Attempts to localize happiness or sadness to activity in certain brain
regions has been more or less futile. This is because emotions involve complex
circuitry, and many overlapping regions are activated in emotional processing.
Furthermore, the method of emotional induction can cause differing regional
changes. For example, if the method of inducing happiness is having a person
reminisce about a happy time, temporal or hippocampal regions may be more
involved, while viewing emotionally charged stimuli may involve visual cortices
to a larger extent.
One ALE meta-analysis observed increased activity in the superior
temporal gyrus in happiness (versus neutral emotional stimuli) and increased
medial frontal gyrus activity in sadness. Another meta-analysis observed no
difference between happiness and sadness, and found them both to activate the
ACC and areas of the prefrontal cortex. This is likely because they are both
involved in emotional processing. Another Meta analysis reported increased
activity of the subcallosal cingulate cortex in sadness, versus increased
activity in the basal ganglia in happiness. Other studies have utilized
different emotional models compared to the traditional classification of
“happiness” and “sadness”, instead comparing things like general emotional
valence, or approach emotions vs retreat emotions. Complex multiaxis models of
emotions have been used, and classification of emotional systems have also been
used, which puts into perspective the complexity of affective neuroscience.
From a pharmacological point, euphoria, which is in a sense pure
happiness, can be induced by opioids or high doses of psychostimulants. Reward
can also be mediated by physical stimulation of the NAcc. Following this logic,
the euphoria of cocaine has been associated with decreased BOLD of the NAcc (possibly
representing a metabolic change associated with opioid release, not
inactivation). If we view sadness through the lens of depression, which would
be a longer term more intense version of normal everyday sadness, then sadness
involves increased activity of the anterior cingulate cortex, with decreased
activity of the striatum and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. However, these
differences may just represent emotional biases that lead to depression, and
therefore is not a great guide to what a non-pathologically sad brain looks
like.
So in summary, it is difficult to determine or localize sadness or
happiness, given current methodological limitations, as well as the possible
poor classifications of “happiness” and “sadness”. However, from Phan et al,
the knowledge of the association between the NAcc (a region of the striatum)
and reward, and the studies of depression, it can be inferred that sadness
involves increaed activity in the anterior cingulate, and happiness involves
increased activity of the striatum. Chemically, the reward elicited by drugs
correlates not with dopamine release, but with the opioid release in the NAcc
elicited by said drug. So chemically, happiness would be represented by
increased NAcc opioid release, and sadness by the opposite. So in short
·
Happiness=Striatum-increased
activity, NAcc-increased endogenous opioid release
·
Sadness=Anterior Cingulate
Cortex-increased activity, NAcc-decreased endogenous opioid release
Reference: Alexander
Saytsev
No comments:
Post a Comment